People's Stories Freedom

View previous stories


Stepping into other people’s shoes has been a catalytic force for social change
by Roman Krznaric
Open Democracy
 
You can always tell when a good idea has come of age: people start criticising it. That’s certainly the case when it comes to empathy.
 
Empathy is a more popular concept today than at any time since the eighteenth century, when Adam Smith argued that the basis of morality was our imaginative capacity for “changing places in fancy with the sufferer.” Neuroscientists, happiness gurus, education policy-makers and mediation experts have all been singing its praises.
 
This has, of course, got the critics charging in, led by Yale psychologist Paul Bloom, who claims that “empathy is biased.” In his view, empathy is a dangerous emotional force that draws us towards identifying with the suffering of particular individuals or our nearest and dearest, while we blithely ignore the plight of distant strangers or people outside our tribe, be it based on religion, ethnicity or class. It also fails to confront the structural barriers to social change.
 
Philosopher Peter Singer takes a similar position in his latest book, The Most Good You Can Do. He cites a study in which one group of people were shown a photo of a single child, with her name and age, and were asked to donate money for a $300,000 drug treatment to save her life. Another group were shown photos (with names and ages) of eight children, and told $300,000 was required for medication that would save all their lives.
 
The result? People gave more to the single child, which is an “absurd outcome” in Singer’s view. His conclusion is that “emotional empathy” biases us towards individual cases, whereas a more rational utilitarian approach—what he calls “effective altruism”—would prompt us to save the larger number of children. It is reason and argument, not empathy he contends, that should be our primary moral guide.
 
This sounds logical, but I believe the anti-empathy brigade is badly mistaken, for two main reasons.
 
First, they show an astonishing willingness to ignore different types of empathy. A standard psychology textbook reveals that there are two forms. One is ‘affective’ empathy, which is about feeling or mirroring others’ emotions—as when Bill Clinton famously told an HIV/AIDS activist that “I feel your pain.” The other is ‘cognitive’ or ‘perspective-taking’ empathy, where you focus on imagining what it’s like to be another person, with their beliefs, experiences, hopes, fears and views of the world.
 
Bloom and Singer only give attention to affective empathy (which they describe as ‘emotional empathy’). I agree with them that a strong emotional resonance can make us unfairly favour individual cases, so of course we should save the eight lives rather than one. But while they both acknowledge the existence of cognitive empathy, they fail to explore its power to promote ethical behaviour.
 
In doing so, what they really miss is that cognitive empathy has been a crucial force in the struggle for human rights and the task of shifting social and political structures. Let me give an example: the campaign against slavery and the slave trade in eighteenth-century Britain.
 
In the 1780s, at a time when half a million slaves were being worked to death on British sugar plantations in the Caribbean, opponents of slavery launched an empathy-based political campaign to get members of the public to understand what it might be like to be a slave. They printed tens of thousands of copies of a poster showing how many slaves could be squeezed onto a slave ship, published oral testimonies of violence against slaves, and got former slaves to give public talks about their ordeals. In other words, they were tapping into the cognitive empathy of some sections of British society.
 
The results were spectacular: public protests, parliamentary petitions, and the world’s first fair trade boycott (of slave-produced sugar). According to historian Adam Hochschild in his book Bury the Chains, the campaign—when combined with other factors such as slave revolts on plantations and structural shifts like the diminishing profitability of the slave economy—played a key role in the abolition of the slave trade in 1807 and the eventual abolition of slavery itself.
 
Hochschild injects a missing ingredient into this story by showing that there was a “sudden upswelling” of human empathy, remarkable for the fact that “it was the first time a large number of people became outraged, and stayed outraged for many years, over someone else’s rights.”
 
The end of slavery illustrates a larger historical pattern: cognitive empathy cracks open the door of moral concern for neglected or marginalised groups, and rights and laws wedge that door wide open. This has happened over and over again since the eighteenth century—in the struggle for civil rights, gay rights, and the rights of women, indigenous people, and disabled people.
 
The key is ‘perspective taking’—trying to imagine what it might be like to be ‘the other’—which makes us care about the plight of those outside our immediate community and treat them as human beings of equal value to ourselves. Political instruments such as public policy and human rights legislation typically play the role of codifying and universalising this moral concern.
 
I put this point to Singer in a public discussion we had recently in Oxford. Doesn’t the slavery case, I asked him, show that ‘reason’—in the form of laws and rights—actually works hand in hand with cognitive empathy to produce the kind of ethical world we both care about? He paused and then said hesitantly, ‘Well, yes, slavery is a good example. Cognitive empathy can make a difference.’
 
In my view, thinkers such as Singer and Bloom are on shaky empirical ground when they put so much faith in the power of reason and rational argument. Even that arch-rationalist Steven Pinker has come round to the importance of cognitive empathy in his vast study of the decline of violence in human history called The Better Angels of Our Nature. Drawing on the work of cultural historians such as Lynn Hunt, he argues that the humanitarian revolution of the eighteenth century—which generated the first campaigns to tackle child poverty, the anti-slavery movement and associations to improve working conditions—was rooted in “the rise of empathy and the regard for human life.”
 
Simply put, taking on the perspective of others is one of the fundamental stepping stones to acknowledging their humanity and inspiring political action. As the cognitive linguist George Lakoff puts it: “Empathy is at the heart of real rationality, because it goes to the heart of our values, which are the basis of our sense of justice. Empathy is the reason we have the principles of freedom and fairness, which are necessary components of justice.” The latest research by leading neuroscientists suggests that he’s right.
 
Cognitive empathy matters because it is part of the toolkit we need to confront the great social, political and ecological challenges of our age. Our failure to act on climate change is, to a significant degree, a failure to step into the shoes of future generations and take into account the impact that our carbon-intensive lifestyles will have on them.
 
The growing wealth inequalities in countries like Britain and the US are fuelled by the failure of the rich and of political elites to step outside the cocoons of their privileged lifestyles and understand what it might be like to be someone lining up at a food bank or having their home threatened with repossession by a bank. And we can never have a cogent or just debate about the influx of immigrants into the European Union until we hear the voices of refugees squeezed onto death-trap boats in the Mediterranean.
 
Ultimately, the place to start creating a more empathic civilisation is in the education system. We need to teach empathy skills to young people so that they develop the deep sense of social and ecological justice that will motivate them to be active citizens —and as a new report from Friends of the Earth reveals, there’s plenty of evidence this can be done. Filling their heads with a list of moral rules from religious texts or rationalist writings isn’t enough. Empathy, especially its cognitive form, is one of the most powerful ways we have of escaping the boundaries of our egoistic concerns, changing our values, and inspiring social action.
 
The historical truth is that reason without empathy is potentially lethal—just think of the Nuremberg Laws that were underpinned by the apparently ‘rational’ racist ideology of the Untermensch, a term meaning ‘subhuman’ that was used to denigrate Jews and Roma. We certainly shouldn’t reject reason. But if we care about forging a world of social and political justice we have to give equal weight to expanding our empathetic imaginations.
 
* Roman Krznaric’s new book is titled Empathy: Why It Matters, and How to Get It. He has taught sociology and politics at Cambridge University and City University, in London: http://www.romankrznaric.com/about


Visit the related web page
 


Time to tackle the social inequalities keeping millions of Australians unwell
by Sharon Friel
Professor of Health Equity, ANU
Australia
 
The poorest 20 per cent of the Australian population can still expect to die on average six years earlier than the richest 20 per cent.
 
For a country that prides itself on a fair go, Australia is almost fatally sick.
 
Today, millions of people''s lives are worsened or unnecessarily shortened by what experts call the social determinants of health inequity: dirty politics, unfair economic arrangements and poor public policies.
 
In a prosperous country like Australia, which has enjoyed 24 years of continued economic growth, is it fair that, as Labor MP Andrew Leigh highlights in his new book Battlers and Billionaires, the poorest 20 per cent of the population can still expect to die on average six years earlier than the richest 20 per cent. Or that Indigenous Australians life expectancy is still 11 years shorter than other Australians.
 
As the Committee for Economic Development of Australia reported earlier this week, more than two million Australians live in poverty. In our cities, suburban areas are marked by concentrations of very disadvantaged residents, from whom long-term unemployment, low levels of education, poor and dangerous housing conditions and, in some situations, extreme poverty lead to higher risk of chronic diseases, such as diabetes, heart disease and cancers, as well as depression.
 
It does not have to be like this. It isn''t a case of poor genetics or even lifestyle choices: the existence of systematic social differences in health outcomes shows there''s something in our society creating an unequal distribution of opportunity to be healthy. These health differences are avoidable, they are created by people and are policy choices.
 
Every public policy, not just health policy, has the potential to affect human health and wellbeing.
 
For example public transport infrastructure investments like the light rail project in the ACT matter for health. Because socially disadvantaged Australians are more reliant on public transport, urban planning decisions that don''t take their needs into account weigh heavier on them.
 
In the ACT, socially disadvantaged people trying to access public transport battle with the infrequency of accessible bus services, especially outside peak times and on weekends, and a lack of understanding and sensitivity by drivers to the needs of different groups of people.
 
Health inequity is also a challenge created in our highest halls of power. Two of the cornerstone policies of the federal government''s 2014 budget put socially-caused health inequity into clear focus: a proposed $7 co-payment for a visit to the doctor and increased cost of medicines; and the now defeated university fee deregulation proposal, with its inevitable increases in fees for students.
 
Both of these policies would have disadvantaged lower socio-economic households, keeping them on a different life trajectory that exposed them to greater health risks.
 
Public policies generate and distribute power, income, goods and services.
 
These are distributed unequally depending on who you are and where you live, and will affect the conditions in which you are born, grow, live, work, age and die. Together these factors are the social determinants of health inequity.


 

View more stories

Submit a Story Search by keyword and country Guestbook